SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.31 issue4Sources of bias in genetic association studies in cattle: A reviewA step-up ractopamine feeding program for finishing pigs author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Revista Colombiana de Ciencias Pecuarias

Print version ISSN 0120-0690

Rev Colom Cienc Pecua vol.31 no.4 Medellín Oct./Dec. 2018

https://doi.org/10.17533/udea.rccp.v31n4a03 

Original Article

Social behavior and group growth of finishing pigs with divergent social breeding values

Comportamiento social y crecimiento grupal de cerdos en etapa de finalización con valores divergentes de crianza sociales

Comportamento social e crescimento grupal de porcos de engorda com valores divergentes de criação social

Joon-Ki Hong 1  

Ki-Hyun Kim 1  

Na-Rae Song 1  

Taejeong Choi 1  

Hyun-Su Hwang 2  

Jae-Kang Lee 2  

Tae-Kyung Eom 2  

Shin-Jae Rhim 2   * 

1Swine Science Division, National Institute of Animal Science, Rural Development Administration, Cheonan 31000, Republic of Korea.

2School of Bioresource and Bioscience, Chung-Ang University, Ansung 17546, Republic of Korea.


Abstract

Background:

Behavioral traits of pigs have been shown to be partly under genetic control, which raises the possibility that behavior might be altered by genetic selection, resulting in pigs with better growth performance.

Objective:

To evaluate the behavior and growth of finishing pigs and investigate pigs selected for high or low social breeding value (SBV) in relation to social behavior and group growth.

Methods:

Thirty-five females and 35 boars from five positive and five negative SBV groups of finishing pigs were grown from 30 to 90 kg and housed in 10 test pens (3.0 × 3.3 m, 7 pigs/pen). Pigs were recorded with video technology for nine consecutive hours on days 1, 15, and 30 after mixing. Pigs were weighed at approximately 90 kg body weight and the number of days to reach 90 kg was then calculated.

Results:

The frequency and duration of behaviors were present in the positive and negative SBV groups after mixing. On day 1 after mixing, agonistic behavior was significantly higher (p=0.027) for the -SBV group compared with the +SBV group. Feeding and feeding-together behaviors were significantly higher (p<0.003) in the +SBV group on days 1 and 30 after mixing. Moreover, growth performance to reach 90 kg body weight was significantly faster (p<0.002) in the +SBV group than in the -SBV group.

Conclusion:

Social interactions, such as feeding-together behavior, among pen mates might affect their growth rate and feed intake. Selection for SBV could be used as an indirect technique for improving growth performance of pigs.

Keywords: agonistic; feed intake; growth performance; mixing; social interactions

Resumen

Antecedentes:

Se ha demostrado que los rasgos conductuales de los cerdos están parcialmente bajo control genético, lo que plantea la posibilidad de que el comportamiento pueda ser alterado vía selección genética y resulte en cerdos con mejores rendimientos de crecimiento.

Objetivo:

Evaluar el comportamiento y crecimiento de los cerdos en etapa de finalización e investigar cerdos seleccionados por un valor alto o bajo de crianza social (SBV) en relación al comportamiento social y al crecimiento grupal.

Métodos:

Treinta y cinco hembras y 35 verracos, pertenecientes a cinco grupos positivos y cinco grupos negativos de SBV de cerdos en etapa de finalización, llevados hasta los 90, desde 30 kg de peso, alojados en 10 corrales de prueba (3,0 x 3,3 m, 7 cerdos/corral). Los cerdos fueron observados con la ayuda de tecnología de vídeo por nueve horas consecutivas en los días 1, 15 y 30 luego de ser mezclados. Además, los cerdos se pesaron a los 90 kg de peso aproximadamente y se calculó el número de días para alcanzar dicho peso.

Resultados:

La frecuencia y duración de los comportamientos de los cerdos en la etapa de finalización se presentaron en los grupos de SBV negativos y positivos luego de ser mezclados. El día 1 luego de la mezcla, el comportamiento agonístico fue significativamente mayor (p=0,027) en el grupo -SBV que en el grupo +SBV. Los comportamientos de consumo de alimento y de consumo en compañía fueron significativamente mayores (p<0,003) en el grupo +SBV en los días 1 y 30 luego de la mezcla. Además, el crecimiento para alcanzar 90 kg de peso corporal fue significativamente más rápido (p=0,002) en el grupo +SBV que el grupo -SBV.

Conclusiones:

Las interacciones sociales, tales como el comportamiento de consumo de alimento en compañía, entre los compañeros de corral, pueden afectar la tasa de crecimiento y consumo de alimento. La selección por SBV podría usarse como técnica indirecta para mejorar el rendimiento de crecimiento en cerdos.

Palabras clave: agonista; consumo de alimento; desempeño de crecimiento; interacciones sociales; mezcla

Resumo

Antecedentes:

Os traços comportamentais dos porcos demonstraram estar parcialmente sob controle genético, o que aumenta a possibilidade de que o comportamento possa ser alterado pela seleção genética e resulte em porcos com melhor comportamento de crescimento.

Objetivo:

Avaliar o comportamento e o crescimento dos porcos de engorda e investigar os porcos selecionados para alto ou baixo valor de reprodução social (SBV) em relação ao comportamento social e crescimento do grupo.

Métodos:

Trinta e cinco fêmeas e 35 machos, pertencentes a cinco grupos de SBV positivos e cinco negativos de porcos de engorda, foram engordados até 90 de 30 kg e alojados em 10 currais de teste (3,0 × 3,3 m, 7 porcos/curral). Os porcos foram observados com o auxílio de tecnologia de vídeo durante nove horas consecutivas nos dias 1, 15 e 30 após a mistura. Além disso, os porcos foram sopesados em aproximadamente 90 kg de peso corporal e o número de dias para atingir 90 kg foi então calculado.

Resultados:

A frequência e a duração dos comportamentos dos porcos de engorda foram apresentadas com grupos de SBV positivo e negativo após a mistura. No dia 1 após a mistura, o comportamento agonístico foi significativamente maior (p=0,027) no grupo -SBV do que no grupo +SBV. Os comportamentos de alimentação e alimentação conjunta foram significativamente maiores (p<0,003) no grupo +SBV nos dias 1 e 30 após a mistura. Além disso, o comportamento de crescimento do grupo para atingir 90 kg de peso corporal foi significativamente mais rápido (p<0,002) no grupo +SBV do que no grupo -SBV.

Conclusão:

As interações sociais, como o comportamento de alimentação conjunta, entre companheiros de curral podem afetar a taxa de crescimento e a ingestão alimentar. A seleção para SBV pode ser uma técnica indireta para melhorar o comportamento de crescimento dos porcos.

Palavras-chave: agonístico; desempenho de crescimento; ingestão de alimentos; interações sociais; mistura

Introduction

Pigs raised in the same pen sometimes show aggressive behaviors toward each other. Aggression is acknowledged as a critical problem, especially after mixing, in commercial pig farming (D’Eath et al., 2009; Turner, 2011). Increases in stress levels, physical activity, and injuries generally occur because of aggression after mixing. This aggression is associated with a reduction in the rate and efficiency of body mass gain, poor meat quality, and low carcass grading (Turner et al., 2009). Despite these disadvantages, mixing at various ages is commonly practice in many countries (Turner et al., 2008).

The estimated breeding value based on individual performance and pedigree information is generally used in genetic evaluation and selection of livestock (Muir, 2005). Genetic selection has mainly focused on reproductive traits and production, such as litter size and daily body mass gain in pigs (Løvendahl et al., 2005). New breeding models include not only individual performance, but also social breeding values (SBV) regarding performance with pen mates. The genetic effects of an individual on the phenotypes of its social partners (for example, pen-mates) are known as SBV (Moore et al., 1997). Moreover, SBVs represent specific traits such as growth rate or feed conversion (Bijma et al., 2007; Reimert et al., 2014). Social breeding values have been observed in several livestock species, including pig (Bergsma et al., 2008), chicken (Ellen et al., 2008), quail (Muir, 2005), deer (Wilson et al., 2011), and mink (Alemu et al., 2014).

Social interactions between individuals have been ignored in animal breeding so far. However, behavioral traits of animals have been shown to be partly under genetic control, which raises the possibility that behavior might be altered by genetic selection, resulting in better adapted animals (D’Eath et al., 2009). The individual growth performance of a pig is affected by its own genes and the genes of its pen mates (Grandinson et al., 2003; Ellen et al., 2008; Reimert et al., 2013a). Social breeding value is related to behaviors such as aggression and competition (Muir, 2005; Wilson et al., 2009). Our hypothesis is that pigs can affect the growth of their pen mates by their own social behavior and through social facilitation of feeding and drinking, increasing food intake and growth.

This study evaluated the behavior and growth of finishing pigs and investigated pigs selected for high or low social breeding value (SBV) in relation to social behavior and group growth.

Materials and methods

Ethical considerations

The experimental protocols describing the management and care of the animals were reviewed and approved according to the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, National Institute of Animal Science, Republic of Korea) on March 7, 2014 (approval number: NIAS 2014-289). Care was taken to minimize pain or discomfort during animal procedures. Drugs commonly administered to deal with animal pain or discomfort -such as tranquilizers, analgesics and anesthetics- were not used in this study.

Location and animals

The study was conducted at the experimental farm of the National Institute of Animal Science in Cheonan (Chungnam Province, South Korea) using Landrace pigs (Chookjin Land/Republic of Korea). Estimated SBVs were based on growth rates during the finishing phase (from approximately 30-90 kg). Social breeding values were estimated with a random regression model for social breeding values in WOMBAT version 1.0 (Meyer, 2007), using the following model that accounted for social genetic effects (Arango et al., 2005; Bijma et al., 2007):

y = Xb + Z D a D + Z S a S + Wl + Vg + e,

where, y is the vector of average daily gain observations; b is the vector of fixed effects, which included batch (year-month), sex (male or female), age at the end of the test, group size, and number of full siblings within the group; a D is the vector of random direct additive genetic effects; a S is the vector of random social genetic effects; l is the vector for the random non-genetic litter effects; g is the vector of random non-genetic group effects (accounting for the group in which the pigs were penned during the finishing period); e is the vector of residuals, and X, Z D , Z S , W, and V are the corresponding incidence matrices.

Assumptions for the probability distributions were ,, and , in which N indicates a normal distribution; I is an identity matrix of the appropriate dimension; and (403 g2/ day2), (95 g2/day2), and (3,875 g2/day2) are the variances of the corresponding effects. Both direct and social additive genetic effects were fitted, which had the following multivariate normal distribution:

where, (2,513 g2/day2) is the variance of direct genetic effects, (9 g2/day2) is the variance of social genetic effects, (55 g/day) is the covariance between direct and social genetic effects, and denotes the Kronecker product.

Dams and sires with the most extreme high and low SBV (+SBV and -SBV, respectively) were selected from the available population to create the test population. Dams (11 sows) were selected form a total of 161 sows and sires (6 boars) were selected form a total of 129 boars (Table 1). Mating between dams and sires was conducted within the same SBV group and pregnant sows were managed under the same conditions (individual housing). The pregnant sows were reared in separate farrowing crates.

Piglets were reared in farrowing crates with solid plastic flooring and a heat lamp. At 28 days of age, piglets were weaned and reared in eight weaning pens (1.8 × 2.4 m, 8-12 piglets/pen) per SBV group. At 30 kg, 70 pigs were allocated to 10 test pens (3.0 × 3.3 m, 7 pigs/pen) that consisted of five pens for each SBV group and were mixed randomly with the SBV groups from the weaning pens. Therefore, there was mixing within SBV groups; however, there was no mixing between +SBV and -SBV groups. The ratio of females/ males was 35/35. The males were not castrated. The environmental conditions were the same in all pens. The temperature was controlled by ventilation fans and heater, and maintained at 28 ± 1 °C. Each pen was provided with a stainless-steel feeder (125 × 35 × 80 cm) and a nipple drinker that allowed the pigs ad libitum access to feed and water throughout the experiment (Rhim et al., 2015).

Wide-angle video cameras were installed at the corners of the test pens (four cameras per pen), so all areas of the pen could be observed. The behaviors of the pigs were video recorded continuously for 9 h/day. All behavioral data were obtained from video images that were digitally recorded from 09:00 to 18:00 h on days 1, 15, and 30 after mixing. Instantaneous scan sampling was carried out at 10-min intervals with the software Vegas Pro ver. 13.0 (Sony, Tokyo, Japan). All video recordings were viewed by trained observers who were blinded to the treatments to eliminate subjective bias and inter-individual discrepancy (Li and Wang, 2011; Rhim, 2012).

The behaviors recorded were drinking, feeding, agonistic, and activity including drinking-together and feeding-together (Table 2). Duration and frequency of behaviors were recorded and the individual pig performing the behavior, as well as the individual pig receiving the agonistic behavior, was noted. The behavioral time values presented are the means and standard errors of the relative frequencies of each behavior, calculated from the results obtained from each observation of each group (Rhim et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2016).

Table 1 Social breeding value (SBV) of parents and test pigs with positive (+) or negative (-) SBV in a Landrace nucleus herd. 

N: number of individuals; SD: standard deviation; -SBV: negative social breeding value; +SBV: positive social breeding value.

Table 2 Ethogram of behavioral categories and their respective definitions (adapted from Statham et al., 2011; Rhim, 2012). 

Pigs were weighed at approximately 90 kg body weight and the data was transformed to the number of days to reach 90 kg using the following calculation (Choi et al., 2013):

Days to 90 kg = age at weighing +

Statistical analysis

Data analysis was performed with RStudio version 0.99.892 (http://www.rstudio.com). The behavioral data were analyzed by a Mann-Whitney U test between the +SBV and -SBV groups using R package coin (Hothorn and Zeileis, 2015). The behavioral differences between days after mixing within each group were assessed with the Kruskal-Wallis test. Days to reach 90 kg were analyzed by a t-test between the +SBV and -SBV groups.

Results

The frequency (Table 3) and duration (Table 4) of behaviors were present in positive (+) and negative (-) SBV groups after mixing. For frequency and duration of drinking behavior, there were no significant differences between the +SBV and -SBV groups during the study period. However, the -SBV group had lower frequency compared with the +SBV group (Mann-Whitney U test, Z = -4.36, p<0.001) in drinking-together at day 30 after mixing.

On day 1 after mixing, the feeding frequency (Z= -2.93, p=0.003) and duration (Z= -2.85, p=0.004) were lower in the -SBV group compared with the +SBV group. On day 15 after mixing, the feeding duration was higher (Z=1.88, p=0.06) in the - SBV group than in the +SBV group. However, feeding frequency was not significantly different between the two groups (Z=0.86, p=0.392). Results reported on day 30 were similar to those obtained on day 1 after mixing, i.e., feeding frequency (Z= -2.91, p=0.003) and duration (Z= -2.43, p=0.014) were lower in the -SBV group than in the +SBV group.

For feeding-together behavior, similar results were obtained as for feeding behavior. The feeding-together frequency (Table 3; days 1 and 30 after mixing) and feeding-together duration (Table 4; days 1 and 30 after mixing) were lower in the -SBV group than in the +SBV group (Z= -5.11 to -4.22, p<0.001). On day 15 after mixing, the feeding-together duration was higher in the -SBV group than in the +SBV group (Z=4.31, p<0.001). There were significant differences in frequency (Kruskal-Wallis test, H=20.87, p<0.001) and duration (H=25.14, p<0.001) of feeding behavior over the three observed days in the -SBV group.

On day 1 after mixing, frequency of agonistic behavior was higher in the -SBV group than in the +SBV group (Mann-Whitney U test, Z=2.21, p=0.027, Table 3) and duration of agonistic behavior was significantly different between the -SBV and +SBV groups (Z=1.81, p=0.050, Table 4). The duration of agonistic behavior of the -SBV group was higher than the + SBV group. However, the frequency and duration of agonistic behavior were not significantly different between the two groups on days 15 and 30 after mixing.

In the -SBV group, the frequency (Kruskal-Wallis test, H=26.68, p<0.001) and duration (H=23.76, p<0.001) of agonistic behavior were significantly different over the three observed days. There were higher frequency and duration of agonistic behavior on day 1 compared with days 15 and 30. Moreover, there were significant differences in the frequency (H=11.41, p=0.003) and duration (H=12.10, p=0.002) of agonistic behavior among days in the +SBV group. Agonistic behavior was higher on day 1 than in days 15 and 30 of the +SBV group (Tables 3 and 4 ).

Table 3 Frequencies of different behaviors of pigs after mixing for groups with positive (+) or negative (-) social breeding value (SBV) in a Landrace nucleus herd; comparisons between groups (n = 5 for + SBV, and n = 5 for -SBV) are based on a Mann-Whitney U test. 

-SBV: negative social breeding value; +SBV: positive social breeding value; SEM: standard error of the mean, Z: value of Mann-Whitney U test.

Table 4 Total duration (sec) spent on different behaviors of pigs after mixing for groups with positive (+) or negative (-) social breeding value (SBV) in a Landrace nucleus herd; comparisons between groups (n = 5 for +SBV, and n = 5 for -SBV) are based on a Mann-Whitney U test. 

-SBV: negative social breeding value; +SBV: positive social breeding value; SEM: standard error of the mean; Z: value of Mann-Whitney U test.

Table 5 Mean days needed for pen mates to reach 90 kg for groups with positive (+) or negative (-) social breeding value (SBV) in a Landrace nucleus herd; comparisons between groups (n = 5 for +SBV, and n = 5 for -SBV) are based on a t-test. 

-SBV: negative social breeding value; +SBV: positive social breeding value; SEM: standard error of the mean.

Days to reach 90 kg was significantly different between the -SBV (135 days) and +SBV (128 days) groups (t-test, t=3.20, p=0.002, Table 5). This means growth was faster in the +SBV group than in the -SBV group. Moreover, there were significant differences in the summation of the days to reach 90 kg of pen mates between the -SBV (812 days) and +SBV (771 days) groups (t = 5.75, p<0.001).

Discussion

Commercially-housed pigs are selected for fast growth rates and maintained in competitive and aberrant behavioral conditions (Rodenburg and Turner, 2012). Aberrant behavior is known to harm health and growth of pigs, and is considered an welfare problem in swine husbandry (Schrøder-Petersen and Simonsen, 2001). In addition, social interactions of group-housed pigs are important for their health, productivity, and welfare (Camerlink et al., 2013). Social interactions among pen mates might affect their welfare and performance considerably, in both negative and positive ways (Reimert et al., 2014). In previous studies, positive SBVs for growth were shown to fear-related behavioral traits in suckling piglets (Reimert et al., 2013b). Moreover, pigs selected for SBVs on the growth of their pen mates exhibited less non-reciprocal biting (Camerlink et al., 2015).

Behavior was affected by SBVs for growth in the present study. Pigs in the +SBV group affected the growth of their pen mates more than those in the -SBV group did. SBVs might have profound effects on response to selection and heritable variation in traits (Moore et al., 1997; Camerlink et al., 2013). Behavioral observations revealed differences in agonistic behavior between negative (-) and positive (+) SBV groups. There was higher frequency and duration of agonistic behavior on day 1 in the -SBV than in the +SBV group. Agonistic behavior was not different at later time points once the groups had settled in this study.

However, Canario et al. (2012) found that the more intense aggression was observed at mixing in the +SBV groups with more stable dominance relationship later and faster growing. Moreover, mixing aggression was not different, but on return to a familiar group after 24 hours away while mixed with unfamiliar pigs, the aggression at reunion was less (Camerlink et al., 2015). This may suggest that social memory is improved. Genetic relationships seem to show that high aggression at mixing can subsequently lead to more stable groups (Desire et al., 2015).

A tentative hypothesis for why +SBV pigs showed less agonistic behaviors when they were mixed with unfamiliar pigs than -SBV pigs could be related to dominant relationships (Canario et al., 2012; Camerlink et al., 2013). Therefore, +SBV pigs could establish their dominant relationship with less agonistic behaviors after mixing. In the present study, +SBV pigs showed less biting, head thrusting, ramming, and pushing other pigs especially on day 1 after mixing. In addition, fearfulness is known as an important factor for social interactions. If pigs with reduced aggression and fearfulness of their pen mates are selected, this might have positive consequences in terms of welfare (Reimert et al., 2014).

We hypothesized that SBV could be related to social behavior and growth performance of finishing pigs. The results suggest that pigs with +SBV and -SBV for growth differed in their social behavior response (Camerlink et al., 2015), and +SBV had a positive effect on the growth of their pen mates. In addition, feeding-together and drinking-together behaviors were observed in this study. A real direct effect could be a greater social facilitation of feeding and drinking in the +SBV animals. Decisions to engage in feeding, feeding-together, and agonistic behaviors might be made by pigs based on the relative benefits and costs of behavior, which will vary depending on production efficiency and animal welfare. Moreover, selection for SBVs that have a positive effect on group growth with pen mates could be used as an indirect technique for improving growth performance and animal welfare (Reimert et al., 2014).

Pigs with divergent SBVs showed differences in social behavior. Social interactions, such as feeding- together behavior, among pen mates might affect their growth rate and feed intake. Selection for SBV could be used as an indirect technique for improving growth performance of pigs. In the present study, only a small- scale experiment was applied to behavioral differences in pigs. Knowledge regarding the mechanisms of social genetic effects might assist with optimal breeding and farming of pigs. Further research is needed using +SBV or -SBV pigs to investigate the behavior and growth of their pen mates.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the technical assistants for their skilled work and the farm staff for animal care at the National Institute of Animal Science. This study was supported by the Agriculture Science and Technology Project (Grant Number: PJ009971042018) of the Rural Development Administration, Republic of Korea.

References

Alemu SW, Bijma P, Moller SH, Janss L, Berg P. Indirect genetic effects contribute substantially to heritable variation in aggression- related traits in group-housed mink (Neovison vison). Genet Sel Evol 2014; 46:11. [ Links ]

Arango J, Misztal I, Tsuruta S, Culbertson M, Herring W. Estimation of variance components including competitive effects of large white growing gilts. J Anim Sci 2005; 83:1241-1246. [ Links ]

Bergsma R, Kanis E, Knol EF, Bijima P. The contribution of social effects to heritable variation in finishing traits of domestic pigs (Sus scrofa). Genetics 2008; 178:1559-1570. [ Links ]

Bijma P, Muir WM, van Arendonk AM. Multilevel selection 1. quantitative genetics of inheritance and response to selection. Genetics 2007; 175:277-288. [ Links ]

Camerlink I, Turner SP, Bijma P, Bolhuis JE. Indirect genetic effects and housing conditions in relation to aggressive behaviour in pigs. Plos One 2013; 8:e65136. [ Links ]

Camerlink I, Ursinus WW, Bijma P, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE. Indirect genetic effects for growth rate in domestic pigs alter aggressive and manipulative biting behaviour. Behav Genet 2015; 45:117-126. [ Links ]

Canario L, Turner SP, Roehe R, Lundeheim N, D’Eath RB, Lawrence AB, Knol EF, Bergsma R, Rydhmer L. Genetic associations between behavioral traits and direct-social effects of growth rate in pigs. J Anim Sci 2012; 90:4706-4715. [ Links ]

Choi JG, Cho CI, Choi IS, Lee SS, Choi TJ, Cho KH, Park BH, Choy YH. Genetic parameter estimation in seedstock swine population for growth performances. Asian-Australas J Anim Sci 2013; 26:470-475. [ Links ]

D’Eath RB, Roehe R, Turner SP, Ison SH, Farish M, Jack MC, Lawrence AB. Genetics of animal temperament: aggressive behavior at mixing is genetically associated with the response to handling in pigs. Animal 2009; 3:1544-1554. [ Links ]

Desire S, Turner SP, D’Eath RB, Doeschl-Wilson AB, Lewis CRG, Roehe R. Analysis of the phenotypic link between behavioural traits at mixing and increased long-term social stability in group- housed pigs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2015; 166:52-62. [ Links ]

Ellen ED, Visscher J, van Arendonk AM, Bijma P. Survival of laying hens: genetic parameters for direct and associative effects in three purebred layer lines. Poult Sci 2008; 87:233-239. [ Links ]

Grandinson K, Rydhmer L, Strandberg E, Thodberg K. Genetic analysis of on-farm tests of maternal behavior in sows. Livest Prod Sci 2003; 83:141-151. [ Links ]

Hothorn T, Zeileis A. Partykit: a modular toolkit for recursive partitioning in R. J Mach Learn Res 2015; 16:3905-3909. [ Links ]

Hwang HS, Lee JK, Eom TK, Son SH, Hong JK, Kim KH, Rhim SJ. Behavioral characteristics of weaned piglets mixed in different groups. Asian-Australas J Anim Sci 2016; 29:1060-1064. [ Links ]

Li Y, Wang L. Effects of previous housing system on agonistic behaviors of growing pigs at mixing. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2011; 132:20-26. [ Links ]

Løvendahl P, Damgaard LH, Nielsen BL, Thodberg K, Su G, Rydhmer L. Aggressive behavior of sows at mixing and maternal behavior are heritable and genetically correlated traits. Livest Prod Sci 2005; 93:73-85. [ Links ]

Meyer K Meat & Livestock Australia and Animal Genetics and Breeding Unit. WOMBAT: a program for mixed model analyses by restricted maximum likelihood., University of New England, Armidale, Australia; 2007. [ Links ]

Moore AJ, Brodie ED, Wolf JB. Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process: 1. direct and indirect genetic effects of social interactions. Evolution 1997; 51:1352-1362. [ Links ]

Muir WM. Incorporation of competitive effects in forest tree of animal breeding programs. Genetics 2005; 170:1247-1259. [ Links ]

Reimert I, Bolhuis JE, Kemp B, Rodenburg TB. Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. Physiol Behav 2013a; 109:42-50. [ Links ]

Reimert I, Rodenburg TB, Ursinus WW, Duijvesteijn N, Camerlink I, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE. Backtest and novelty behavior of female and castrated male piglets, with diverging social breeding values for growth. J Anim Sci 2013b; 91:4589-4597. [ Links ]

Reimert I, Rodenburg TB, Ursinus WW, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE. Responses to novel situations of female and castrated male pigs with divergent social breeding values and different backtest classifications in barren and straw-enriched housing. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2014; 151:24-35. [ Links ]

Rhim SJ. Effects of group size on agonistic behaviors of commercially housed growing pigs. Rev Colomb Cienc Pecu 2012; 25:353-359. [ Links ]

Rhim SJ, Son SH, Hwang HS, Lee JK, Hong JK. Effects of mixing on the aggressive behavior of commercially housed pigs. Asian- Australas J Anim Sci 2015; 28:1038-1043. [ Links ]

Rodenburg TB, Turner SP. The role of breeding and genetics in the welfare of farm animals. Anim Front 2012; 2:16-21. [ Links ]

Schrøder-Petersen DL, Simonsen HB. Tail biting in pigs. Vet J 2001; 162:196-210. [ Links ]

Statham P, Green L, Bichard M, Mendl M. A longitudinal study of the effects of providing straw at different stages of life on tail-biting and other behavior in commercially housed pigs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2011; 134:100-108. [ Links ]

Turner SP. Breeding against harmful behaviors in pigs and chickens: state of the art and the way forward. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2011; 134:1-9. [ Links ]

Turner SP, Roehe R, D’Eath RB, Ison SH, Farish M, Jack MC, Lundeheim N, Rydhmer L, Lawerence AB. Genetic validation ofpostmixing skin injuries in pigs as an indicator of aggressiveness and the relationship with injuries under more stable social conditions. J Anim Sci 2009; 87:3076-3082. [ Links ]

Turner SP, Roehe R, Mekkawy W, Farnworth MJ, Knap PW, Lawrence AB. Bayesian analysis of genetic associations of skin lesions and behavioural traits to identify genetic components of individual aggressiveness in pigs. Behav Genet 2008; 38:67-75. [ Links ]

Wilson AJ, Gelin U, Perron MC, Réale D. Indirect genetic effects and the evolution of aggression in a vertebrate system. Proc Royal Soc B Biol Sci 2009; 276:533-541. [ Links ]

Wilson AJ, Morrissey MB, Adams MJ, Walling CA, Guinness FE, Pemberton JM, Clutton-Brock TH, Kruuk LEB. Indirect genetics and evolutionary constraint: an analysis of social dominance in red deer, Cervus elaphus. J Evol Biol 2011; 24:772-783. [ Links ]

Received: August 01, 2017; Accepted: June 14, 2018

* Corresponding author: Shin-Jae Rhim, School of Bioresource and Bioscience, Chung-Ang University, Ansung 17546, Republic of Korea. Tel.: +82-31-670-4842. Fax: +82-31-676-4842. E-mail: sjrhim@cau.ac.kr

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare they have no conflicts of interest with regard to the work presented in this report.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License