SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.18 issue2Supporting Migrant Fencers in a Sporting EnvironmentThe Relationship Between Body Mass Index and the Physical Activity Level in Adolescents author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Pensamiento Psicológico

Print version ISSN 1657-8961

Pensam. psicol. vol.18 no.2 Cali Jul./Dec. 2020  Epub Dec 30, 2020

https://doi.org/10.11144/javerianacali.ppsi18-2.pemp 

Artículos

Predicting Exertion from Metabolic, Physiological and Cognitive Variables in Older Adults*

Predicción del esfuerzo con variables metabólicas, fisiológicas y cognitivas en adultos mayores

Predição do esforço com variáveis metabólicas, fisiológicas e congênitas em idosos

1Universidad de Costa Rica, San Pedro (Costa Rica)

2Universidad de Costa Rica, San Pedro (Costa Rica)

3Universidad de Costa Rica, San Pedro (Costa Rica)

4Universidad de Costa Rica, San Pedro (Costa Rica)

5University of California, Davis (USA)

6University of Kansas, Lawrence (USA)

7University of Kansas, Lawrence (USA)

8University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City (USA)

9Universidad de Costa Rica, San Pedro (Costa Rica)

10Universidad de Costa Rica, San Pedro (Costa Rica)


Abstract

Objective.

To determine the association between perceived exertion (RPE) and physiological variables and to determine the predictive factors of RPE during submaximal and maximal exertion in older adults.

Method.

Older adults from Kansas (n = 100) and Costa Rica (n = 79) performed a submaximal and maximal stress test. RPE, education level and a total score of cognitive function (CFTS) were collected. Correlation and multiple regression analyses were computed using RPE as the criterion variable and oxygen consumption (VO2), respiratory exchange ratio (RER), CFTS and education level as predictors.

Results.

There was a significant correlation between VEP and RER (r = 0.22, p = 0.029) at maximal exertion. The RPE predictors for women were VO2, RER, and CFTS at different stages of the test. For men, VEP was predicted by RER, education level, and VO2 at different stages.

Conclusion.

Metabolic and physiological variables predicted RPE in older adults. Age, cognition, and heart rate were unrelated to RPE at maximal exertion, and RER predicted RPE scores during submaximal and maximal exertion.

Keywords Perception; respiration; older adults; testing

Resumen

Objetivo.

Determinar la asociación entre el esfuerzo percibido (VEP) y variables fisiológicas y determinar los factores predictivos de VEP durante el esfuerzo submáximo y máximo en adultos mayores.

Método.

Adultos mayores de Kansas (n = 100) y Costa Rica (n = 79) realizaron una prueba de esfuerzo submáxima y máxima. Se recopiló la VEP, nivel de educación y una puntuación total de la función cognitiva (PTFC). Se usaron análisis de correlación y regresión múltiple utilizando VEP como variable criterio y el consumo de oxígeno (VO2), tasa de intercambio respiratorio (RER), PTFC y nivel de educación como predictores.

Resultados.

Se encontró una correlación significativa entre la VEP y la RER (r = 0.22, p = 0.029) en el esfuerzo máximo. Los predictores de VEP para las mujeres fueron VO2, RER y PTFC en diferentes etapas de la prueba. Para los hombres, la VEP fue predicha por RER, nivel de educación y VO2 en diferentes etapas.

Conclusión.

Las variables metabólicas y fisiológicas predijeron los valores de VEP en adultos mayores. La edad, la cognición y la FC no se relacionaron con la VEP en el esfuerzo máximo y la RER predijo las puntuaciones de la VEP durante el esfuerzo submáximo y máximo.

Palabras clave Percepción; respiración; adultos mayores; pruebas

Resumo

Escopo.

Determinar a associação entre o esforço percebido (VEP) e as variáveis fisiológicas e determinar os fatores preditivos de VEP durante o esforço submáximo e máximo em idosos.

Metodologia.

Idosos de Kansas (n= 100) e Costa Rica (n= 79) realizaram uma prova de esforço submáxima e máxima. Foi recopilada o VEP, nível de educação e pontuação total da função cognitiva (PTFCO. Foram usadas análises de correlação e regressão múltipla utilizando VEP como variável critério e o consumo de oxigeno (VO2), taxa de intercambio respiratório (RER), PTFC e nível de educação como preditores.

Resultados.

Foi encontrada uma correlação significativa entre a VEP e a RER (r = 0.22, p = 0.029)) no esforço máximo. Os preditores de VEP para as mulheres foram VO2, RER e PTFC em diferentes etapas da prova. Para os homens, a VEP foi predita por RER, nível de educação e VO2 em diferentes etapas.

Conclusão.

As variáveis metabólicas e fisiológicas predisseram os valores de VEP em idosos. A idade, a cognição e a FC não estiveram relacionadas com a VEP no esforço máximo e a RER predizia as pontuações da VEP durante o esforço submáximo e máximo.

Palavras-chave Percepção; respiração; idosos; provas

Introduction

Exercise prescription for older adults is often based on relative intensities of measured variables such as heart rate (HR), oxygen consumption (VO2), a maximal muscle repetition, and cognitive measures such as ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) (American College of Sports Medicine, 2018; Chodzko-Zajko et al., 2009; Zaleski et al., 2016). These parameters are measured during a graded exercise test (GXT) and provide objective and accurate information for practitioners. However, these indicators are usually difficult to measure, require trained staff, medical supervision, and expensive equipment. Therefore, alternative measures such as the rating of perceived exertion (RPE), are recommended as perceptual measures associated to physiological stress.

The RPE might represent a biomarker of fatigability associated with the progressively lower physical activity occurring with aging (Wanigatunga et al., 2018). There are numerical and pictorial RPE scales for children and adults (Chen, Chiou, Tzeng, Lu, & Chen, 2017; Eston, Parfitt, Campbell, & Lamb, 2000; Robertson et al., 2000; Williams, Eston, & Furlong, 1994); however, the first RPE scale developed was Borg’s (Borg, 1970, 1990). This scale was developed assuming a linear association between physiological markers (e.g., HR, blood lactate) and the participant’s effort perception at any given exercise intensity (Borg, 1970).

The validity of Borg’s RPE has been questioned since inconsistencies in the strength of the association were found between this perceptual measure and physiological markers (Chen, Fan, & Moe, 2002). The strength of the association between HR, blood lactate, VO2, maximal attained VO2 (VO2max), highest attained VO2 (VO2peak), respiratory exchange ratio (RER), and other markers measured during exercise ranged between 0.57 and 0.64. These correlation coefficients were lower than the 0.80 to 0.90 reported before; therefore, moderator variables such as previous fitness, type of exercise protocol, population, and others must be taken into consideration when assessing RPE during a GXT (Chen et al., 2002).

Aging is strongly associated to cognitive decline, which is partially explained by cardiovascular health and other associated disease risk factors (Barnes, 2015). One of the most noticeable physiological change during aging is a decrease in VO2max (Wilson & Tanaka, 2000), which declines at a rate of 5% to15% per decade after 25 years of age in sedentary males and females regardless of their ethnic background (Åstrand, Bergh, & Kilbom, 1997; Schiller, Casas, Desouza, & Seals, 2001). The study of cardiovascular function is relevant since a high VO2max is related to a lower reduction in cortical tissue density in the brain (Colcombe et al., 2003) and an increased brain volume (gray and white matter regions) (Colcombe et al., 2006); thus, the brain might become more efficient, plastic and adaptive to exercise training, reducing the normal cognitive decline associated with the aging process (Colcombe et al., 2004; Kramer & Colcombe, 2018).

Aerobic exercise training has been positively related to a delayed cognitive impairment (Fiuza-Luces, Garatachea, Berger, & Lucia, 2013) through positive changes in brain structure and function (Firth et al., 2018; Kennedy, Hardman, Macpherson, Scholey, & Pipingas, 2017). Similar brain responses have been reported for resistance training exercise in older adults with mild cognitive impairment (Tsai, Ukropec, Ukropcová, & Pai, 2018). However, changes in cognition in the aging brain might influence RPE during GXTs, a topic not extensively reported in the literature. Equivocal results have been found in the scarce evidence regarding the association between physiological measures and RPE in adults over 60 years old. For instance, positive correlations were found between HR and RPE during a cycle ergometer GXT in female (Shigematsu, Ueno, Nakagaichi, Nho, & Tanaka, 2004) and male and older female adults (Guidetti et al., 2011). However, no associations were found in an arm training GXT on a sample of older female adult participants (Grange et al., 2004). The evidence also suggests that RPE is trainable (Grange et al., 2004; Groslambert & Mahon, 2006).

Although, others have reported on the association between RPE and physiological responses during cycle ergometry GXT in older adults (Shigematsu et al., 2004). To the best of our knowledge, no study has reported the associations between metabolic, physiological and cognitive variables and RPE during a stage-by-stage treadmill GXT in older adults. Therefore, the purpose of the study was to explain the contribution of metabolic, physiological and cognitive variables on RPE during a GXT in older adults. Two research questions led our work; first, what would be correlation between RPE and physiological variables during submaximal (i.e., stage-by-stage) and maximal effort? Secondly, what would be the RPE predictors during submaximal and maximal GXT? We hypothesized that RPE would be moderate-to-highly related to HR, VO2, RER at both, maximal and submaximal (i.e., stage-by-stage) effort. In addition, we hypothesized that RPE could be predicted by physiological and cognitive variables during submaximal and maximal GXT.

Methods

Participants

Two data sets were analyzed from an ongoing comparative study in older adults from Kansas and Costa Rica. For the maximal effort analysis (study 1), volunteers were 100 older adults who were given a testing appointment at the Alzheimer’s Disease Center at the University of Kansas. For submaximal or stage-by-stage analysis (study 2), volunteers were 79 older adults who were given a testing appointment at the Human Movement Sciences Center at the University of Costa Rica, Costa Rica.

In both testing sites, the respective medical team determined whether volunteers were eligible for the study based on their personal health history, a physical examination and an interview. All participants were screened and allowed to participate if they were at least 60 years old, scored in the unimpaired range on the cognitive status screen, and had visual, auditory, and physical abilities sufficient to complete the cognitive and exercise assessments. For safety precautions, volunteers with severe electrocardiographic, intellectual, metabolic or gait abnormalities were excluded from participating in the study. Participants read and signed a written informed consent previously approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Kansas and the Scientific Ethics Committee at the University of Costa Rica.

Instruments

For study 1 and study 2, the same neurocognitive battery was applied through a personal interview, performed by licensed psychologists. The highest education level attained was asked as part of the demographics interview. The neurocognitive battery assessed different cognition domains, including a cognitive screen, simple speed of processing, spatial visualization, visuospatial processing (fluid ability), episodic memory and verbal abilities, executive functioning and cognitive control, and working memory (simple attention) tasks.

The cognitive screen was the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). The simple speed of processing was measured with the Stroop test (Stroop, 1935), which was used to assess executive function. The spatial visualization was measured with the Space Relations Test (SRT; Bennet, Seashore, & Wesman, 1972). The visuospatial processing/fluid ability was measured with the Block Design Test (BDT; Wechsler, 1997). The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST; Wechsler, 1997), was used to assess cognitive processing ability. The Trail-making Test (TMT) forms A and B (Armitage, 1946), was used to assess information regarding visual search, scanning, speed of processing, mental flexibility, and set switching as an executive function. The episodic memory and verbal abilities were measured with the Boston Naming Test (BNT; Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983), a measure of visual confrontational word retrieval using black and white line drawings of progressive difficulty. Logical and delayed memory (Wechsler, 1997) were measured by a prose recall of short narrative passages immediately after the auditory presentation the story (reading out loud), and then prose recall of short narrative passages after a 20-min delay of the auditory presentation the story (reading out loud). The executive functioning and cognitive control were measured with the TMT (form B) and the Stroop Test (interference task), were used to assess executive function and cognitive control. In addition, verbal fluency (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983) was measured by a verbal test which consists of saying as many words as possible from a category in one minute. Finally, the working memory (simple attention) was measured with the Digit Span forwards and backwards (DSF involves reciting back a list of numbers to read to the subject, DSB involves reciting back a list of numbers in reverse order) and the Letter Number Sequencing (LNS) tests was used to assess working memory (Wechsler, 1997).

Anthropometric measures recorded were body height (cm), weight (kg), body fat mass (%) and bone mineral density (BMD) by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA), using a Lunar Prodigy Advance equipment (GE Medical Systems, Madison, WI). The measurements were obtained following international standards (American College of Sports Medicine, 2018; International Society for Clinical Densitometry, 2015). Aerobic power as determined by VO2, HR, and RER (VCO2/VO2) were measured during the GXT with metabolic carts (TrueOne 2400, Parvomedics, Sandy, UT, for study 1 and Jaeger CPX, CareFusion Corp., San Diego, CA, for study 2). The RPE was assessed by the 6-20 Borg’s scale (Borg, 1970, 1990) during each stage of the GXT.

Procedures

Participants were recruited by word of mouth and telephone calls in both testing sites. For the neurocognitive assessment, the psychologists received participants in quiet rooms, where they sat quietly and were given the option to drink a beverage and eat a light snack during the length of the testing session, which lasted approximately 90-min, allowing for breaks when needed. The individual cognitive dimensions measured were transformed in z-scores and participant’s mean performance z-scores on each dimension were computed to create a comprehensive index of global neurocognitive performance called cognitive function total score (CFTS). This composite score was used as a predictor for statistical purposes.

Body height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a stadiometer and body mass was measured in kg on a digital platform. For DXA assessment, participant’s scans were performed and analyzed on each testing facility by the same-trained operator (Exercise Science specialist), according to a standard protocol (International Society for Clinical Densitometry, 2015; Lewiecki et al., 2016; Morgan & Prater, 2017). In both testing locations, exercise was performed at room temperature ranging from 22°C to 25°C between 7:00 am to 9:00 am.

The VO2, HR, and RER were measured by a GXT using a modified Cornell treadmill protocol. Exercise science specialists and physicians asked participants to walk at 2.73 km/h at 0% incline and expired gases were collected breath-by-breath on the metabolic cart. The treadmill grade, speed or both increased every 2-min until VO2peak was achieved (Hawkins, Raven, Snell, Stray-Gundersen, & Levine, 2007). At the end of each GXT stage, RPE was asked to participants, and a physician continuously monitored the 12-lead electrocardiogram to measure HR and detect any rhythm abnormalities. Systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure were measured at each stage of the GXT, and mean arterial pressure (MAP) was calculated as follows: 0.33 x SBP + 0.66 x DBP.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed with the IBM-SPSS Statistics, version 22 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, New York). Descriptive statistics are presented as mean and standard deviation (M ± SD), unless otherwise noted. For all inferential statistical analyses, the level of significance was set a priori at p ≤ 0.05 and 95% confidence intervals around the estimate were computed. For study 1 (i.e., maximal exertion), a non-parametric independent samples Mann-Whitney U test was computed on RPE at maximal exertion. Based on this analysis, Pearson correlations were computed between RPE and HRmax, VO2peak, RER, CFTS, and education level. Stepwise multiple linear regression analysis was computed using RPE as the criterion variable, and HRmax, VO2peak, RER, CFTS, and education level as predictor variables. For study 2 (i.e., stage exertion), non-parametric independent samples Mann-Whitney U tests were computed on each stage of the GXT on the dependent variable RPE. Based on these analyses, Pearson correlations were computed between RPE and physiological variables HR, SBP, DBP, MAP, VO2 and RER by gender at each GXT stage. Finally, stepwise multiple linear regression analyses by gender at each GXT stage were computed using RPE as the criterion variable, and HR, VO2, RER, CFTS and education level as the predictor variables.

Results

Descriptive statistics for older adults from Kansas and Costa Rica are presented in table 1. For study 1, participants were 65 female and 35 male older adults. The median RPE values were similar between males (Mdn = 18, range = 7) and females (Mdn = 17, range = 8) at maximal exertion (p = 0.288); therefore, data from male and females were collapsed for further analyses. A significant correlation was found between RPE and RER (r = 0.22, p = 0.029), and non-significant correlations were found between RPE and HRmax (r = 0.18, p = 0.068), VO2peak (r = 0.18, p = 0.077), CFTS (r = -0.02, p = 0.825), and education level (r = 0.06, p = 0.581). The stepwise multiple linear regression analysis showed that RPE was explained only by RER (Standardized β = 0.21, p = 0.039, CI95% = 0.27, 10.52, SEE = 1.74, R2 = 0.03). Predictors HRmax (p = 0.153), VO2peak (p = 0.211), CFTS (p = 0.774), and education level (p = 0.422) did not explain RPE scores.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for Kansans and Costa Rican older adults. Values are M ± SD 

BMI = Body Mass Index; BMD = Bone Mineral Density; CFTS = Cognitive Function Total Score.

Source: Own elaboration.

For study 2, participants were 53 female and 26 male older adults (Table 1). Descriptive statistics for physiological and perceptual variables at each GXT stage are presented in table 2.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics (M ± SD) by gender for each GXT stage in Costa Rican older adults 

RPE = Rating of Perceived Exertion; HR = Heart rate in beats per minute (bpm); SBP = Systolic Blood Pressure; DBP = Diastolic Blood Pressure; MAP = Mean Arterial Pressure; VO2 = Oxygen Consumption; RER = Respiratory Exchange Ratio.

Source: Own elaboration.

Significant differences were found between male and female older adults on RPE scores on GXT stage 1 (p = 0.006), stage 2 (p = 0.001), stage 3 (p = 0.003), stage 4 (p = 0.008), and stage 5 (p = 0.005). RPE scores were similar between males and females on stage 6 (p = 0.120). No between-gender comparisons were computed on stages 7, 8 and 9 given the small sample size (Table 2). Pearson correlation coefficients between RPE and physiological variables by gender are shown in figures 1 and 2.

Source: Own elaboration.

Figure 1. Pearson correlation coefficients between heart rate (HR) and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) at each stage of a gradual maximal exercise test (GXT) in Costa Rica female (A) and male (B) older adults. *p ≤ 0.05. 

Source: Own elaboration.

Figure 2. Pearson correlation coefficients between mean arterial pressure (MAP), oxygen consumption (VO2) and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) at each stage of a gradual maximal exercise test (GXT) in Costa Rican female (A1, A2) and male (B1, B2) older adults. *p ≤ 0.05. 

Significant multiple regression predictors of RPE for females were VO2 (R2 = 0.24) for stage 1 and stage 6 (R2 = 0.69), RER (R2 = 0.28) and CFTS (R2 = 0.07) for stage 2, and RER for stages 3 (R2 = 0.45), 4 (R2 = 0.26), and 5 (R2 = 0.22). For males, the significant multiple regression predictors of RPE were RER for stages 1 (R2 = 0.26), 2 (R2 = 0.65), and 3 (R2 = 0.53), RER (R2 = 0.42) and Education level (R2 = 0.13) for stage 4, and RER (R2 = 0.33), VO2 (R2 = 0.18) and Education level (R2 = 0.18) for stage 5 (Table 3).

Table 3 Multiple linear regression coefficients on physiological, metabolic and cognitive predictors of ratings of perceived exertion in Costa Rican female and male older adults 

VO2: oxygen consumption; RER: respiratory exchange ratio; CFTS: cognitive function total score; Education: Education level.

Source: Own elaboration.

Discussion

The aims of the study were to determine the association between RPE and physiological and cognitive variables, as well as to identify RPE predictors at submaximal and maximal effort during a treadmill GXT. We analyzed samples of older adults from Kansas for maximal effort (study 1) and Costa Rica for submaximal or stage-by-stage GXT (study 2).

The main finding in the older adult sample from Kansas (study 1) was that RPE at maximal exertion during the GXT was similar between males and females. In addition, only the metabolic variable RER was a significant predictor of the RPE scores. Other physiological (i.e., HRmax, VO2peak) and cognitive (i.e., CFTS, education level) predictors were unrelated to RPE scores at maximal exertion.

The lack of association between RPE and physiological variables is consistent with previous meta-analytic findings (Chen et al., 2002). Several potential moderators might impact the strength of the association between HR and RPE; for instance, fitness level, exercise type, and exercise protocol (Groslambert & Mahon, 2006). According to Chen et al. (2002), a stronger association between HR and RPE has been found on sedentary persons, compared to healthy but inactive, and active and highly fit individuals. However, others have found a lack of association between HR and RPE in sedentary participants (Grange et al., 2004). In addition, higher correlation coefficients between HR and RPE have been also found on swimmers compared to track runners, treadmill exercise, and cycle ergometry participants. Finally, lower correlation coefficients between HR and RPE have been found on random intermittent protocols compared to progressive continuous or intermittent protocols, submaximal and maximal exertion protocols (Chen et al., 2002). In the present study, we assessed mostly sedentary older adults on a treadmill to maximal exertion; therefore, we expected to find a strong correlation between HR and RPE; however, we did not find it at maximal exertion.

The RPE scales are useful since participants learn how to relate physiological and metabolic responses with a felt whole-body perception of effort, decreasing the need for frequent HR measurements (Powers & Howley, 2018). Grange et al. (2004), suggest that sedentary older adults could not perceive their body sensations (e.g., muscular strain, aches and fatigue) properly or that they are less accurate at rating them during intensive exercise such as the maximal GXT used in the present study. However, RPEs are changed following six-weeks of arm training, which might be explained by the older adult’s ability to enhance their muscular sensory cues in the perception of effort (Grange et al., 2004). The changes might have been mediated by increased blood perfusion to the brain and the action of a family of neurotrophins and neurotransmitters associated to brain plasticity and cognitive function (e.g., brain derived neurotrophic factor – BDNF, dopamine, serotonin) (Jimenez-Maldonado, Renteria, Garcia-Suarez, Moncada-Jimenez, & Freire-Royes, 2018).

Others suggest that in older adults the differences in perceptual responses may not be present as long as variations in aerobic power are taken into account (Groslambert & Mahon, 2006). This impaired kinetic perception hypothesis explanation is plausible as supported by a body of correlational studies on the reduced sensory and sensorimotor abilities occurring during normal aging (Li & Lindenberger, 2002; Schneider & Pichora-Fuller, 2000). However, more experimental studies are required to determine the causal effects of aging on perceptual responses using different theoretical approaches such as the information degradation hypothesis (Monge & Madden, 2016) and the teleoanticipation hypothesis (Hampson, St Clair Gibson, Lambert, & Noakes, 2001).

The main finding in the older adult sample from Costa Rica (study 2) was that RPEs were significantly lower in males compared to females during the first five out of six stages of the GXT analyzed. In the last stage, scores between males and females were similar. Only a few older adults completed stages seven, eight, and nine (Table 2); therefore, the data were not analyzed to avoid low statistical power. The analyzed data suggested that males and females perceive exertion differently during submaximal exercise, and the difference is lost at or near-maximal effort. The lower RPE in males might be explained by a sociological concept of machismo in that men, in following stricter roles of traditional masculinity, may be more influenced to prove effortlessness, virility and emotional toughness compared to women (Gibbons & Luna, 2015). There is evidence showing that male athletes have higher mental toughness compared to female athletes (Newland, Newton, Finch, Harbke, & Podlog, 2013; Nicholls, Polman, Levy, & Backhouse, 2009).

Inconsistent low-to-moderate significant correlation coefficients were found between HR and RPE at initial stages of the GXT in males and females (Figure 1). Low- to-moderate significant correlation coefficients were found between MAP and RPE only in female older adults (Figure 2, A1), and between VO2 and RPE in male and female older adults (Figure 2, A2, B2).

In conclusion, in the present study in older adults from Kansas and Costa Rica, we expected a strong correlation between HR and RPE based on literature suggesting such an association (Groslambert & Mahon, 2006). However, we did not find that association at maximal exertion in older adults from Kansas. We found low-to-moderate correlation coefficients between HR and RPE at the initial stages of the GXT in the Costa Rican sample of older adults. In addition, the RPE were different between males and females at or near maximal effort.

Aerobic exercise is strongly recommended to older adults given the positive impact on brain tissue preservation and plasticity (Colcombe et al., 2003; Colcombe et al., 2006; Colcombe et al., 2004; Firth et al., 2018; Kramer & Colcombe, 2018). The RPE is recommended for monitoring aerobic exercise intensity in the absence of objectively measured variables, such as HR (Groslambert & Mahon, 2006). However, in the older adults in this study, RPE was inconsistently related to HR, yet, consistently related to the metabolic parameter RER, which can only be measured by expensive laboratory equipment. Therefore, the validity of the RPE may not be as high in older adults as it might be for other populations, and its widespread use should be carefully evaluated.

The respiratory rate (breaths×min-1), a highly correlated variable to RER, could be the best indicator of physical intensity or exertion since meta-analytic evidence has shown coefficients above 0.70 when correlated to RPE (Chen et al., 2002). Respiratory rate might be used for tracking exercise intensity and exertion since breaths are easy to count (Chen et al., 2002), and are closely related to RER. Hyperventilation or increased respiratory rate occurs when hydrogen ions are elevated due to a progressive exertion, which is explained by excessive CO2 loss from the body’s CO2 stores. Since RER is defined as the ratio of CO2 produced to the O2 consumed (VCO2/VO2; Powers & Howley, 2018), then respiratory rate should be considered a valid and reliable estimator of exercise intensity and exertion in older adults regardless of their fitness status. Thus, new scales for monitoring different exercise modalities (i.e., aerobic, anaerobic) need to be developed to properly control the training progression and safety in older adults (e.g., Colado et al., 2018).

Acknowledgments

This project was registered at the University of Costa Rica’s Vice-Presidency of Research under the code VI-838-B4-364. The authors would like to thank Jorge Ramírez-Morales, Marcela Martí-Revelo, Aleida Ortega-Meléndrez, Mario Villarreal-Ángeles, Rodrigo Cordero-Tencio, and Valeria Pagonas-Moncada for their help in logistics, data collection and processing for this study in Costa Rica.

References

American College of Sports Medicine. (2018). ACSM's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription (10th ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. [ Links ]

Armitage, S. G. (1946). An Analysis of Certain Psychological Tests Used in the Evaluation of Brain Injury. Psychological Monographs, 60(1), 1-48. doi: 10.1037/h0093567 [ Links ]

Åstrand, P. O., Bergh, U., & Kilbom, A. (1997). A 33-yr Follow-Up of Peak Oxygen Uptake and Related Variables of Former Physical Education Students. Journal of Applied Physiology, 82(6), 1844-1852. doi:10.1152/jappl.1997.82.6.1844 [ Links ]

Barnes, J. N. (2015). Exercise, Cognitive Function, and Aging. Advances in Physiology Education, 39(2), 55-62. doi:10.1152/advan.00101.2014 [ Links ]

Bennet, G. K., Seashore, H. G., & Wesman, A. G. (1972). Differential Apititudes Tests Space Relations Form T (5th ed.). New York: Harcourt Educational Measurement/Psychological Corporation. [ Links ]

Borg, G. (1970). Perceived Exertion as an Indicator of Somatic Stress. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 2(2), 92-98. doi: 10.2340/1650197719702239298 [ Links ]

Borg, G. (1990). Psychophysical Scaling with Applications in Physical Work and the Perception of Exertion. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 16(Suppl 1., 55-58. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1815. [ Links ]

Chen, M. J., Fan, X., & Moe, S. T. (2002). Criterion-Related Validity of the Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale in Healthy Individuals: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(11), 873-899. doi:10.1080/026404102320761787 [ Links ]

Chen, Y. L., Chiou, W. K., Tzeng, Y. T., Lu, C. Y., & Chen, S. C. (2017). A Rating of Perceived Exertion Scale Using Facial Expressions for Conveying Exercise Intensity for Children and Young Adults. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 20(1), 66-69. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2016.05.009 [ Links ]

Chodzko-Zajko, W. J., Proctor, D. N., Fiatarone, M. A., Minson, C. T., Nigg, C. R., Salem, G. J., & Skinner, J. S. (2009). Exercise and Physical Activity for Older Adults. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 41(7), 1510-1530. doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181a0c95c [ Links ]

Colado, J. C., Pedrosa, F. M., Juesas, A., Gargallo, P., Carrasco, J. J., Flandez, J., ... Naclerio, F. (2018). Concurrent Validation of the OMNI-Resistance Exercise Scale of Perceived Exertion with Elastic Bands in the Elderly. Experimental Gerontology, 103, 11-16. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2017.12.009 [ Links ]

Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K. I., Raz, N., Webb, A. G., Cohen, N. J., McAuley, E., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Aerobic Fitness Reduces Brain Tissue Loss in Aging Humans. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 58(2), 176-180. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.2.m176. [ Links ]

Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P. E., Kim, J. S., Prakash, R., McAuley, E., ... Kramer, A. F. (2006). Aerobic Exercise Training Increases Brain Volume in Aging Humans. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 61(11), 1166-1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166. [ Links ]

Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J., ... Elavsky, S. (2004). Cardiovascular Fitness, Cortical Plasticity, and Aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(9), 3316-3321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400266101 [ Links ]

Eston, R. G., Parfitt, G., Campbell, L., & Lamb, K. L. (2000). Reliability of Effort Perception for Regulating Exercise Intensity in Children Using the Cart and Load Effort Rating (CALER) Scale. Pediatric Exercise Science, 12(4), 388-397. doi: 10.1123/pes.12.4.388 [ Links ]

Firth, J., Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., Schuch, F., Lagopoulos, J., Rosenbaum, S., & Ward, P. B. (2018). Effect of Aerobic Exercise on Hippocampal Volume in Humans: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. NeuroImage, 166, 230-238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.007 [ Links ]

Fiuza-Luces, C., Garatachea, N., Berger, N. A., & Lucia, A. (2013). Exercise is the Real Polypill. Physiology, 28(5), 330-358. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00019.2013 [ Links ]

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-Mental State". A Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [ Links ]

Gibbons, J. L., & Luna, S. E. (2015). For Men Life is Hard, for Women Life is Harder: Gender Roles in Central America. In S. Safdar & N. Kosakowska-Berezecka (Eds.), Psychology of Gender Through the Lens of Culture: Theories and Applications (pp. 307-325). Cham: Springer International Publishing. [ Links ]

Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). Boston Naming Test scoring booklet. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. [ Links ]

Grange, C. C., Maire, J., Groslambert, A., Tordi, N., Dugue, B., Pernin, J.-N., & Rouillon, J.-D. (2004). Perceived Exertion and Rehabilitation with Arm Crank in Elderly Patients After Total Hip Arthroplasty: A Preliminary Study. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 41(4), 611-620. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2003.05.0080. [ Links ]

Groslambert, A., & Mahon, A. D. (2006). Perceived Exertion: Influence of age and Cognitive Development. Sports Medicine, 36(11), 911-928. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200636110-00001 [ Links ]

Guidetti, L., Sgadari, A., Buzzachera, C. F., Broccatelli, M., Utter, A. C., Goss, F. L., & Baldari, C. (2011). Validation of the OMNI-Cycle Scale Of Perceived Exertion in the Elderly. Journal of Aging & Physical Activity, 19(3), 214-224. doi: 10.1123/japa.19.3.214. [ Links ]

Hampson, D. B., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. I., & Noakes, T. D. (2001). The Influence of Sensory Cues on the Perception of Exertion During Exercise and Central Regulation af Exercise Performance. Sports Medicine, 31(13), 935-952. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200131130-00004 [ Links ]

Hawkins, M. N., Raven, P. B., Snell, P. G., Stray-Gundersen, J., & Levine, B. D. (2007). Maximal Oxygen Uptake as a Parametric Measure of Cardiorespiratory Capacity. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 39(1), 103-107. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000241641.75101.64 [ Links ]

International Society for Clinical Densitometry. (2015). Official Positions 2015 ISCD Combined (adult & pediatric). Middletown, CT: ISCD. [ Links ]

Jimenez-Maldonado, A., Renteria, I., Garcia-Suarez, P. C., Moncada-Jimenez, J., & Freire-Royes, L. F. (2018). The Impact of High-Intensity Interval Training on Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor in Brain: A Mini-Review. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 839. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00839 [ Links ]

Kennedy, G., Hardman, R. J., Macpherson, H., Scholey, A. B., & Pipingas, A. (2017). How Does Exercise Reduce The Rate of Age-Associated Cognitive Decline? A Review of Potential Mechanisms. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 55(1), 1-18. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160665. [ Links ]

Kramer, A. F., & Colcombe, S. (2018). Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study-Revisited. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 213-217. doi: 10.1177/1745691617707316 [ Links ]

Lewiecki, E. M., Binkley, N., Morgan, S. L., Shuhart, C. R., Camargos, B. M., Carey, J. J., ... Leslie, W. D. (2016). Best Practices for Dual-Energy X-ray Absorptiometry Measurement and Reporting: International Society for Clinical Densitometry Guidance. Journal of Clinical Densitometry, 19(2), 127-140. doi: 10.1016/j.jocd.2016.03.003 [ Links ]

Li, K. Z. H., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Relations Between Aging Sensory/Sensorimotor and Cognitive Functions. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 777-783. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00073-8 [ Links ]

Monge, Z. A., & Madden, D. J. (2016). Linking Cognitive and Visual Perceptual Decline in Healthy Aging: The Information Degradation Hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 166-173. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.031 [ Links ]

Morgan, S. L., & Prater, G. L. (2017). Quality in Dual-Energy X-Ray Absorptiometry Scans. Bone, 104, 13-28. doi: 10.1016/j.bone.2017.01.033 [ Links ]

Newland, A., Newton, M., Finch, L., Harbke, C. R., & Podlog, L. (2013). Moderating Variables in the Relationship between Mental Toughness and Performance in Basketball. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 2(3), 184-192. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2012.09.002 [ Links ]

Nicholls, A. R., Polman, R. C. J., Levy, A. R., & Backhouse, S. H. (2009). Mental Toughness in Sport: Achievement Level, Gender, Age, Experience, and Aport Type Differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(1), 73-75. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.02.006 [ Links ]

Powers, S. K., & Howley, E. T. (2018). Exercise physiology: Theory and Application to Fitness and Performance (10th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Education. [ Links ]

Robertson, R. J., Goss, F. L., Boer, N. F., Peoples, J. A., Foreman, A. J., Dabayebeh, I. M., ... Thompkins, T. (2000). Children's OMNI Scale of Perceived Exertion: Mixed Gender and Race Validation. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 32(2), 452-458. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200002000-00029. [ Links ]

Schiller, B. C., Casas, Y. G., Desouza, C. A., & Seals, D. R. (2001). Maximal Aerobic Capacity Across Age in Healthy Hispanic and Caucasian Women. Journal of Applied Physiology, 91(3), 1048-1054. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.91.3.1048 [ Links ]

Schneider, B. A., & Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2000). Implications of Perceptual Deterioration for Cognitive Aging Research. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.). New Jersey: Erlbaum. [ Links ]

Shigematsu, R., Ueno, L. M., Nakagaichi, M., Nho, H., & Tanaka, K. (2004). Rate of Perceived Exertion as a Tool to Monitor Cycling Exercise Intensity in Older Adults. Journal of Aging & Physical Activity, 12(1), 3-9. doi: 10.1123/japa.12.1.3 [ Links ]

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651 [ Links ]

Tsai, C. L., Ukropec, J., Ukropcová, B., & Pai, M. C. (2018). An Acute Bout of Aerobic or Strength Exercise Specifically Modifies Circulating Exerkine Levels and Neurocognitive Functions in Elderly Individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment. NeuroImage: Clinical, 17, 272-284. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.10.028 [ Links ]

Wanigatunga, A. A., Simonsick, E. M., Zipunnikov, V., Spira, A. P., Studenski, S., Ferrucci, L., & Schrack, J. A. (2018). Perceived Fatigability and Objective Physical Activity in Mid- to Late-Life. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 73(5), 630-635. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx181 [ Links ]

Wechsler, D. (1997). Adminstration and scoring manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. San Antonio: Harcourt Brace. [ Links ]

Williams, J. G., Eston, R., & Furlong, B. (1994). CERT: A Perceived Exertion Scale for Young Children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79(3 Pt 2), 1451-1458. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.79.3f.1451 [ Links ]

Wilson, T. M., & Tanaka, H. (2000). Meta-Analysis of the Age-Associated Decline in Maximal Aerobic Capacity in Men: Relation To Training Status. American Journal of Physiology. Heart and Circulatory Physiology, 278(3), H829-834. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.2000.278.3.H829 [ Links ]

Zaleski, A. L., Taylor, B. A., Panza, G. A., Wu, Y., Pescatello, L. S., Thompson, P. D., & Fernandez, A. B. (2016). Coming of Age: Considerations in the Prescription of Exercise for Older Adults. Methodist DeBakey Cardiovascular Journal, 12(2), 98-104. doi: 10.14797/mdcj-12-2-98 [ Links ]

*The National Institutes of Health - Fogarty (1R21TW009665–01) and intramural research funds from the University of Costa Rica supported this work. The National Institutes of Health under award numbers R01 AG034614, UL1 TR000001, KL2 TR000119, P30 AG035982, and K01 HD067318 supported portions of this research. Additional support was provided by the Foundation for Physical Therapy New Investigator Fellowship Training Initiative at the University of Kansas.

Received: June 10, 2019; Accepted: August 26, 2020

1 Ph.D. in Physical Activity and Sport Sciences. https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8070-1111.

2 M.Sc. in Human Movement Sciences. https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3497-7410.

3 Bachelor in Physical Education. https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2175-9609.

4 Lic. in Psychology. https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7815-9618.

5 Ph.D. in Clinical Psychology. https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4262-8173.

6 Ph.D. in Gerontology. https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8385-1091.

7 Ph.D. in Physical Therapy. https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5181-7131.

8 Ph.D. in Rehabilitation Science. https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1618-7207.

9 Ph.D. in Clinical Neuropsychology. https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9311-8195.

10 Ph.D. in Biomedical Sciences. Human Movement Sciences Research Center (CIMOHU), University of Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica. P.O. Box 239-1200 Pavas, San José, Costa Rica. Tel. +506 2511-2909, E-mail: jose.moncada@ucr.ac.cr https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9807-5163.

Creative Commons License This work is under License Creative Commons Attribution-NonComercial-No Derivates 4.0 Internacional.