SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.15 issue4Spanish Adaptation of the HPLP-II toward with a Sample of EmployeesConstruct Validity of the WISC-IV among a Brazilian Sample author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Universitas Psychologica

Print version ISSN 1657-9267

Univ. Psychol. vol.15 no.4 Bogotá Oct./Dec. 2016

https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-4.rjsn 

The Relationship between Junior and Senior Nurses. Analysis of a Case of Intergenerational Discrimination Using the RepMut Tool*

Las relaciones entre enfermeras junior y senior. Análisis de un caso de discriminación intergeneracional utilizando la herramienta RepMut

Philippe Castel**
Université Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France

Carlos Roberto Velandia-Coustol***
Université Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France

Florent Mangin
Université Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France

Audrey Peteuil
Université Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France

Marie Jegu****
Université Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France

Marie-Françoise Lacassagne*****
Université Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France


*Research paper.

Author notes
**Professor of social psychology, SPMS Laboratory, University of Bourgogne-Franche Comté (France). Email: Carlos.Velandia@u-bourgogne.fr
***
Associated researcher – SPMS Laboratory.
****
PhD Student.
*****
Professor of social psychology and Director of SPMS Laboratory

Received: 21 October 2015Accepted: 15 July 2016


How to cite:

Castel, Ph., Velandia-Coustol, C. R., Mangin, F., Peteuil, A., Jegu, M., & Lacassagne, M. -F. (2016). The relationship between junior and senior nurses. Analysis of a case of intergenerational discrimination using the RepMut Tool. Universitas Psychologica, 15 (4). http://dx.doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-4.rjsn


Abstract

This research aims to show that the implementation of intergenerational salience at a social level can affect the work context, particularly in hospitals. Participants were 50 voluntary nurses (25 juniors, 25 seniors) working in French hospitals. RepMut Methodology, based on the theories of social categorization and self-categorization, highlights the mutual representations of group relations and allows measurement of the biases of perception and evaluation. The results support the idea that junior/senior self-categorization plays an important role in structuring work relations. Both groups express in-group homogeneity, but also contrast and values consistent with their social group membership. We discuss the benefit of differentiating the levels of analysis in order to acquire an understanding of diversity and its associated effects.

Keywords : ageism, identity strategies, RepMut, social categorization.


Resumen

Esta investigación considera el impacto social de las diferencias intergeneracionales en contexto laboral hospitalario. Participaron voluntariamente 50 enfermeros(as) de hospitales públicos franceses (25 junior, 25 senior). La metodología RepMut, basada en las teorías de la categorización social y de la autocategorización, permite evidenciar las representaciones mutuas de las relaciones intergrupales y una fácil medición de sesgos perceptivos y evaluativos. Los resultados muestran que la autocategorización Junior/Senior juega un papel importante en la estructuración de las relaciones laborales. Los participantes expresan un efecto de homogeneidad pro-endogrupo, acentuando el contraste y los valores coherentes con su endogrupo. La discusión aborda la ventaja de diferenciar entre niveles de análisis para adquirir una comprensión de la diversidad y sus efectos asociados.

Palabras clave: edadismo, estrategias de gestión de identidad, RepMut, categorización social.


Social context

In a context where aging societies in Western Europe are confronting the world of work to new challenges (Fagnani & Letablier, 2011; Meier, 2008), maintaining seniors in the labour market has become a new and major social issue in France. While the proportion of older people in France keeps growing, the employment rate of older workers – those aged between 55 and 64, the "seniors" – is still well under the European average: 47% in 2014 for France in comparison to 51.9% for the European Union (Statistical Office of the European Union [Eurostat], 2015a). However, a clear progression is to be noted: in 2004, 37.8% of the seniors of active age were employed. This progression can be explained by the increase of the average retirement age and the abolition of measures for attractive early retirement packages (Juban, 2013).

Now, society needs more than ever for its seniors to stay active as long as possible: the expenses of healthcare and pensions as well as the high unemployment rate of juniors (24.2% in 2014) and seniors (7.5 % in 2014, Eurostat, 2015b) cost too much for France. The system faces new challenges, combining an economic perspective and a social and societal point of view: a fear of a shortage of qualified and experienced workers, of a loss of knowledge acquired from experience, and realising the great importance of maintaining the social link between generations and supporting the social balance (Cahuc, 2005; Guillemard, 2007; Juban, 2013; Loisel, 2002; Meier, 2008).

Unlike in Germany, where – because of a decrease of the junior population and the fear of a lack of skilled manpower – encouraging companies to maintain seniors at work and encouraging seniors to stay at work is a major goal for employment professionals and policy makers (Steinberg & Jégu, 2015), French juniors face difficulties of access to initial employment and French seniors, difficulties to stay employed. As a result, on the labour market, the two generations are placed in a relation of competition by the cultural, economic, and social context (Jégu, Lacassagne, & Castel, 2015, 2015). This competitive situation could lead to discrimination. From this perspective, it seems then necessary to study seniors and juniors' relationship and their mutual social representation of it, to restore an intergenerational dialogue and, ultimately, the intergenerational balance.

Intergroup Process

In terms of social psychology, "Social identity theory" (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and "Self-Categorization Theory" (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987) attempted to explain the processes of discrimination.

According to these theories, people consider belonging to their social group as an important part of their own identity. When they adopt a social identity, they try to behave in ways which give a positive and distinct image of their group. When they compare themselves with other groups, they are likely to accentuate the difference between their group and the other group and they are likely to accentuate the similarity of the members of each group. In other words, people see the members of the group to which they belong (the in-group) as being different from the out-group, and members of the same group as being more similar than they are. They also have a tendency to evaluate their own group or its members more favourably than a group to which they do not belong or its members. The tendency to serve the group takes the form of favouring the in-group, not-favouring, or disfavouring the out-group. In general, it would seem that people reinforce their self-esteem in this way.

These phenomena, which are based on social identity, do not appear in other identity-related areas. In fact, social identity is merely a level of identity on a scale enabling a distinction between collective identity in relation to a particular society (super-ordinate level), and personal identity in relation to the uniqueness of each individual (subordinate level). In terms of levels of identity, therefore, there are ways of attenuating or even removing the effects relating to the mobilization of a social identity.

For example, it is possible to help the members of groups in conflict by making them change their identity level, encouraging a super-ordinate level (Sherif, 1954), or encouraging each party to share a common fate. It is also possible to help them to recognize sub-groups in the opposing group (Brauer & Er-rafiy, 2011) to encourage individuation by progressing to the subordinate level. Finally, by remaining at the intermediate level, it is possible to highlight several categories at the same time (cross-categorization) to increase the number of salient dimensions of comparisons (Crisp & Hewstone, 2007; for a detailed review, Deschamps & Doise, 1979).

Current Research

Considering the repartition of the different age groups and the economic crisis, the French labour market is characterised by great difficulties to absorb the population from the two extremes of the age distribution, those old enough to start working (juniors from 18 through 30 years old) and those still allowed to be active (seniors from 55 up to 67 years old). As seen previously, this situation causes an increase of age-based prejudice and potential intergenerational conflict (North & Fiske, 2015). It puts juniors and seniors in a complex relationship of confrontation. The demographic problem actually increases significance of age in work relations. Now, once a social identity is apparent, group members try to enhance the importance of their own group. Consequently, we will find discrimination between generational groups and this discrimination will appear through the bias of perception (difference and similarity) and the bias of evaluation.

However, in the context of French public hospitals, intergenerational conflicts are not prevalent, as nurses share a strong social identity that can unite members regardless of their age (super-ordinate goal).

This identity is reinforced by their shared concerns about health and issues (common fate). Age is evenly spread across different sub-categories of nurses (cross-categorization). For example, in surgery and birth services, the age distribution of nurses is comparable.

Finally, intergenerational relationships do not seem to be in competition (shortage of graduates, job security, age/experience-related wages).

Consequently, this research aims to show that the implementation of intergenerational salience at social level can affect many different contexts, particularly including places that are protected or sheltered from threats, such as hospitals.

Method

Participants and design

Participants were voluntary nurses (N=50, 46 females and 4 males) recruited in public hospitals in a French department. The junior sample (N= 25, 23 females, 2 males) had a mean age of 26.68 years (SD= 2.29) and the senior sample (N= 25, 23 females, 2 males) had a mean age of 53.38 years (SD= 2.72). The design was a 2 (Sources: nurses less than 30 years old / nurses more than 50 years old) between-group factor X 2 (Targets: Junior/Senior) between subjects factor.

Material and Procedure

We used a specific methodology, recently developed by the SPMS Laboratory (University of Burgundy - Franche-Comté), to highlight the views of interacting groups and to reveal mutual social representations. This methodology, called Mutual Representations (RepMut) 1 , is based on the theory of social categorization and self- categorization, and allows an easy measurement of the biases of perception and evaluation. The applications of this tool can be found in different fields (health (Peteuil, 2015), education (Harabi, Mangin, & Castel, 2015; Mangin, 2015), justice (Joly, 2013 enterprise (Jégu et al., 2015), interethnic relations (Velandia-Coustol, Castel, & Lacassagne, 2015), and sports (Harabi, Mangin, Perchot, Castel, & Lacassagne, 2015; Perchot, Mangin, Lacassagne, & Castel, 2013; Perchot, 2013).

The RepMut methodology is an intergroup relations analysis tool, in the form of a questionnaire in its data collection phase. It uses the various advances on the social categorization model, incorporating a part of the methodology of social representations (Abric, 1984; Flament, 2001; Le Bouedec, 1984; Moscovici, 1984) and the notion of co-construction produced from the social psychology of language (Ghiglione, 1988; Ghiglione & Blanchet, 1991).

A research assistant presented a RepMut questionnaire to each participant according to their appearance (junior or senior). They were asked to help with a class project at the university that sought to determine "in the context of an increasing retirement age, the representation of nurses' relationship in the medical field". Anonymity was guaranteed.

The RepMut questionnaire covers different representations: in-group representation (X), out-group representation (Y), meta-representation (what subjects think that members of the other group think about members of their group (X)), stereo-representation (what subject thinks that members of their group think about members of the other group (Y)).

In the current research, to test our hypothesis, only the in-group representation and the out-group representation were studied.

Consenting participants were directly interviewed in their category (junior/senior).

  1. They were asked to produce 5 adjectives describing their opposite category and their own category. This indicator is named "representation".

  2. They were then asked to say to what degree these adjectives were positive, neutral or negative (-100 to +100). This is called the "valence" indicator.

  3. In addition, to rate the adjective valence, participants were asked to rate the strength (-100 to +100) of status. The question was "To what degree does this adjective give access to a high social position (0 to 100)?" This is called the "strength of status" indicator.

  4. They were then asked to rate the % of members of their own group and of the other group defined by each adjective. This is called the "In-/out -group representativity" indicator.

  5. They were also asked to give the proportion of nurses defined by each quality. This is called the "supra-representativity" indicator.

  6. Finally, they were asked to say to what extent each adjective defined them in person. This is called the "self-representativity" indicator.

In this study, to test our hypothesis, only the questions 1, 2, 4 and 5 are analysed. The plan for analysis is as follows:

Boxes 1, 2, 3, 4 correspond to the scores (assimilation, contrast, appreciation) of a source group for a target group. The score in a box corresponds to the average of the marks given by subjects in the source group to the target group, the mark of a subject itself corresponding to the average of the assessments of the 5 adjectives that they provided for this target.

The marginal averages in columns (1+3) and (2+4) correspond to source group norms (nurses under the age of thirty, nurses over the age of fifty). The difference between these averages corresponds to any possible difference in normative terms.

The marginal averages in rows (1+2) and (3+4) correspond to a priori stances concerning the targets (junior/senior). The difference between these averages corresponds to any possible cultural prejudice.

The marginal averages on the diagonal (2+3) and (1+4) correspond to category assessments (in-group/out-group). The difference between these averages corresponds to any possible category-based discrimination.

From a statistical point of view, adopting an Sn <Source2>*Target2 plan, the major effect of the Source Variable (under thirty/over thirty) corresponds to the difference between norms, the major effect of the Target Variable (junior/senior) corresponds to prejudice, and the interaction corresponds to the major effect of the Categorization Variable, being category-based discrimination.

Dependent measure

Measurement of similarity: The assimilation score was the average percentage given by each member of one group to judge the degree of similarity between members of the other group, or between members of their own group. The higher the percentage, the stronger the assimilation: members of the group under consideration are perceived as all similar.

Measurement of contrast: The contrast was the difference between the representativity score for the group under consideration (the group for whom the adjective were given) and the representativity score for the other group.

Measurement of appreciation: The appreciation score was the mean of adjective values (-100 to +100) weighted by the % (0 to 100) of group members defined by these adjectives.

Results

Measurement of similarity

A mixed-measure ANOVA with target group (Junior/Senior) as a within-subject factor and age of the source (<30 years old/ >50 years old) as a between-subject factor revealed an interaction (F (1.46) = 8.55, p <.005). This interaction corresponds to the categorization bias.

There is no difference in norms according to source (under thirty/over fifty) or a priori differences between the targets. On the other hand, there is a categorization effect, the in-group being considered to be more homogenous than the out-group.

Measurement of contrast

Result 1: an ANOVA measure with Representativity (In-group representativity/Out-group representativity) as within-subject factor revealed a significant effect for each group. In all cases, the subjects believe that the adjectives that they have chosen to describe a group are more representative of this group than of the other.

A mixed-measure ANOVA with target group (Junior/Senior) as a within-subject factor and age of the source as a between-subject factor revealed no interaction, no effect of target but a tendential effect of the age corresponding to a norm effect (F (1.46) = 2.865, p < 0.09). This effect comes mainly from the difference between the under-thirties and the over-fifties on the junior target group (F (1.48) = 3.89, p < 0.05). Senior nurses discriminate less against the junior target group than the young ones. The analysis of statistical contrasts also shows that there is no normative effect on the senior target and that, furthermore, in nurses over the age of 50, there is a tendential effect between the juniors and seniors. In tendential terms, members of this age group rate the members of the junior target group as being less contrasting than the senior target group.

Measurement of appreciation

A mixed-measures ANOVA with Target group (In–group/Out-group) as a within-subject factor and Age of the source as a between-subject factor revealed no effect of the Source, no effect of the target, but a significant effect of interaction corresponding to categorization bias (F (1,46) = 10.64, p =.002).

There is no source or target effect. However, there is a categorization effect in which subjects, regardless of their category, prefer their group to the other group. This result is similar to the results for similarity. Shared representativity across the two indicators is at risk of reinforcing the effect, and we have verified that results were maintained on consideration of raw data. The pattern of results is indeed similar. There is no source effect (F (1,46) = 0.40, NS), no target effect (F (1,46) = 0.040, NS), and an interaction effect (F (1,46) = 5.99, p <.02).

The analysis of statistical contrasts shows that nurses under thirty exhibit a difference in appreciation according to target in favour of the juniors (F (1,48) = 7.33, p <.009). Likewise, the analysis reveals a difference in handling by the two sources. The junior target group is rated higher by the junior source group than by the senior source group (F (1.48) = 6.18, p < 0.016).

Discussion

Overall, these results provide additional evidence that junior and senior nurses activate the social categorization process through the biases of perception and evaluation.

Firstly, there is an assimilation effect, junior and senior nurses perceived the members of their own group as more similar than the members of the other group.

This initial result shows that, in terms of assimilation, the perception bias does not depend on the age of the source or the age of the target but on the categorization (in-group/out-group). This categorization effect is marked by a more homogenous view of one's own group than of the other group. In the literature, the effect most commonly recorded concerns the homogeneity of the out-group (Lorenzi-cioldi, 1993, 1998). However, it would seem that the effect is modulated by the respective group status. In fact, a number of studies relating to low-status populations record cases where the homogeneity of the in-group is greater than (Brown & Smith, 1989; Simon, 1992, for example) or equal to that of the in-group (see for a review, Boldry, Gaertner, & Quinn, 2007). Both groups studied occupy highly comparable 2status positions in objective terms, which means in the hospital environment, a subordinate position in the hierarchy. Therefore, it is probable that the marking of categorization by greater homogeneity for the in-group than for the out-group is the most available strategy, given their usual status in the hospital.

Secondly, an observation of the results of the contrasts shows that they are all positive and widely significant. This means that nurses believe that the terms that they have used to describe a group are clearly more representative of this group than the other. In other words, they have a clear view of the specific characteristics of each group.

This inter-group distinction is not based on the characteristics of the target group in terms of either age or categorization. However, such a sense of distinction has a tendency to be greater in junior nurses than in senior ones. The under-30s see juniors and seniors as groups with boundaries, whereas the over-50s see these boundaries as being much more fluid, especially with regard to the juniors. It is as if the senior nurses applied an identity-based strategy (Van Knippenberg, 1989), blurring the boundary separating them from the junior nurses.

Finally, results for appreciation show that there is no source age effect (difference in norms) or target age effect (prejudice). On the contrary, as with similarity, there is a categorization effect. When we fine-tune the data, it would appear that the juniors over-rate their own group (auto-favouritism), whereas the seniors do not.

In conclusion, the juniors over-rate themselves, assimilate each other and identify inter-group contrast, which could suggest the emergence of an elitist strategy (Castel, Lacassagne, & Viry, 2006). On the other hand, the seniors are aware that they belong to a group that sets them apart from the juniors; however, the contrast they see is not so great, and they do not seem to want to be marginalized, even if they do not go as far as showing allo-disfavourism, as do groups that are objectively disfavoured (Castel et al., 2006).

General Discussion

Current research has tested the idea that self- categorization in terms of junior/senior plays an important role in structuring work relations. The research provided clear support for this idea. The answers to the questionnaire showed that junior and senior nurses are motivated to express an in-group homogeneity effect, to express contrast and to express values consistent with their social group membership.

Where generation-related identities are salient, subjects react as members of these categories and try to find validating identities within these groups. In the study presented, the juniors, active in their role as juniors in relation to their senior colleagues, mark themselves out more in their own favour, whereas the nurses over the age of 50 - perhaps because they see themselves as being more representative of nurses as a whole - avoid their own marginalization by playing down differences with the other group, but still without giving them a very favourable assessment.

One limitation of our research is that only a small number of people took part in this research, and senior participants were not really "old people". Nevertheless, this minor incursion at a meso-social level, where groups begin to interact with each other, shows the incidence of social categories in an environment that remains sheltered in terms of differences between generations.

Demographic issues are requiring European societies to reconsider age in terms of economic development in the world of employment. Incidence at meso-social level has every chance of being negative, but this is not irreversible. In other words, if the automatic effects of social categorization are deleterious, there are still ways around it.

An important challenge for professionals concerned by seniors and their work is to give them consideration. Given that the reasons behind discrimination appear to be problems with identity recognition, as suggested by Barreto and Ellemers (2002), it will be possible to lay the foundations of cooperative behaviour between junior and senior workers in order to acknowledge meaningful identities.

Conclusion

The study presented underlines the benefit of differentiating between different levels of analysis in order to acquire an understanding of diversity and its associated effects. Facts observed at macro-social level are not irreversible, in that they result from a meso- social dynamic. In this respect, the appropriation of differences produces spontaneous effects. Interaction between two complementary groups almost automatically results in assimilation, contrast, and discriminating judgements. In a given situation, an analysis of mutual relationships makes it possible to identify particular risks. In our study, the juniors appear to assume the role of an elitist minority, and the seniors that of a dominant majority. If these roles were to become entrenched over time, it is likely that tensions would arise. However, the implementation of regulatory procedures tailored to the type of relationship would make developing innovative working practices possible. Skills differentiation, probably related to generation-based rather than age-based characteristics, should encourage a certain creativity in terms of interpersonal relationships.

Acknowledgement

This research was made possible by the Synerginov program. We thank XXX and XXX for their assistance with data collection and all students of the (university) who participated during their Master to the RepMut elaboration, we are thinking more precisely about XX and XX.


Foot Note

Notes

1Représentations Mutuelles (in French). RepMut is a tool that has been developed based on the design of social partitions by a working group (mainly Représentations Mutuelles (in French). RepMut is a tool that has been developed based on the design of social partitions (Castel & Lacassagne, 2011, 2015) by a working group (mainly Castel, P., Lacassagne, MF, Mangin, F., Peteuil, A., Velandia-Coustol, C.) from the SPMS laboratory.
2The scores obtained from answers to question 3 make it possible to check that subjective statuses are equivalent.


References

Abric, J. C. (1984). L'artisan et l'artisanat: analyse du contenu et de la structure d'une représentation sociale. Bulletin de Psychologie, 37 (15-17), 861-875.         [ Links ]

Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2002). The impact of respect versus neglect of self- identities on identification and group loyalty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 28 (5), 629-639. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167202288007         [ Links ]

Boldry, J. G., Gaertner, L., & Quinn, J. (2007). Measuring the measures: A meta-analytic investigation of the measures of outgroup homogeneity. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations , 10 (2), 157-178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430207075153         [ Links ]

Brauer, M., & Er-rafiy, A. (2011). Increasing perceived variability reduces prejudice and discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 47 (5), 871-881. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.003         [ Links ]

Brown, R., & Smith, A. (1989). Perceptions of and by minority groups: The case of women in academia. European Journal of Social Psychology , 19 (1), 61-75.         [ Links ]

Castel, P., & Lacassagne, M.-F. (2011). Contrat de communication et partitions sociales. En P. Castel, É. Sales-Wuillemin, & M.-F. Lacassagne (Eds.), Psychologie sociale, Communication et Langage (pp. 19-50). Paris, France: De Boeck.         [ Links ]

Castel, P., & Lacassagne, M.-F. (2015). Theory of social partitions and identity dynamics. En B. Mohan (Ed.), Construction of Social Psychology (pp. 93-104). Lisbonne: Portugal: inScience Press. Recuperado a partir de http://insciencepress.org/construction-of-social-psychology/        [ Links ]

Castel, P., Lacassagne, M.-F., & Viry, C. (2006). Biais de discrimination et statut social. Une étude de terrain sur les relations intergroupes. Le travail humain , 69 (4), 305-315.         [ Links ]

Cahuc, P. (2005). Le difficile retour en emploi des seniors. Revue Française D'économie, 1 (20), 3-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/rfeco.2005.1564         [ Links ]

Crisp, R. J., & Hewstone, M. (2007). Multiple social categorization. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 39, pp. 163-254). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)39004-1         [ Links ]

Deschamps, J.-C., & Doise, W. (1979). L'effet du croisement des appartenances catégorielles. In W. Doise (Ed.), Expériences entre groupes (pp. 282-292). Paris: Mouton.         [ Links ]

Eurostat. (2015a, July). Employment (main characteristics and rates) - annual averages (lfsi_emp_a). Retrieved from http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/products-datasets/product?code=lfsi_emp_a         [ Links ]

Eurostat. (2015b, September). Unemployment rates by sex, age and educational attainment level (%) (lfsa_urgaed). Retrieved from
http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/products-datasets/-/lfsa_urgaed         [ Links ]

Fagnani, J., & Letablier, M. -T. (2011). L'enjeu démographique dans les débats et la mise en place des réformes des politiques familiales en France et en Allemagne: 1990-2008. In S. Gouazé, C. Prat-Erkert & A. Salles (Eds.), Les enjeux démographiques en France et en Allemagne, réalités et conséquences . Villeneuve d'Asq, France: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion. Retrieved from https://books.google.fr/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=abCV1Sk62PAC&oi=fnd&pg=PP1#v=onepage&q&f=false         [ Links ]

Flament, C. (2001). Structure, dynamique et transformation des représentations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Ed.), Pratiques sociales et représentations (3rd ed., pp. 37-58). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.         [ Links ]

Ghiglione, R. (1988). Enjeux et discours: le paradigm de la coherence. (Challenges and discourses: the paradigm of coherence). Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale , 1 (3-4), 367-389.         [ Links ]

Ghiglione, R., & Blanchet, A. (1991). Analyse de contenu et contenus d'analyses . Paris: Dunod.         [ Links ]

Guillemard, A.-M. (2007). Pourquoi l'âge est-il en France le premier facteur de discrimination dans l'emploimont? Retraite et Société , 51 (2), 11-25. Retrieved from www.cairn.info/revue-retraite-et-societe-2007-2-page-11.htm         [ Links ]

Harabi, S., Mangin, F., & Castel, P. (2015). Social Partitions and Identity Processes: an illustration in the field of physical education. En XIVth European Congress of Psychology . Milan, Italy.         [ Links ]

Harabi, S., Mangin, F., Perchot, R., Castel, P., & Lacassagne, M.-F. (2015). RepMut: a group processes and intergroup relations diagnostic tool, illustrations in the field of sport and physical activity. En 14th European Congress of Sport Psychology . Bern, Switzerland.         [ Links ]

Jégu, M., Lacassagne, M.-F., & Castel, P. (2015). Wider den Krieg der Generationen in der Arbeitswelt. Kategorisierung und "Partitions Sociales" als Analyseraster für intergenerationelle Beziehungen in Unternehmen - Unterstützungsmöglichkeiten im Bereich Sport. En H. Dill & H. Keupp (Eds.), Der Alterskraftunternehmer. Ambivalenzen und Potenziale eines neuen Altersbildes in der flexiblen Arbeitswelt (pp. 157-182). Bielefeld: Transcript.         [ Links ]

Joly, C. (2013). Sentenced people. What social representation? En C. Pracana & L. Silva (Eds.), International Psychological Applications Conference and Trends, InPACT 2013 . Madrid,Spain: Word Institute for Advanced Research and Science. Retrieved from http://www.inpact-psychologyconference.org/2013/         [ Links ]

Juban, J. Y. (2013). L'entretien d'évaluation du personnel, dispositif clé de l'évolution entre exclusion et intégration des seniors. Revue de Gestion des Ressources Humaines , 88 (2), 33-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/grhu.088.0033         [ Links ]

Le Bouedec, G. (1984). Contribution to the methodology of the study of social representations. Current Psychology of Cognition , 4 (3), 245-272.         [ Links ]

Loisel, J.-P. (2002). Les seniors et l'emploi. L'intérim, une solution ou un facteur aggravant de précarisation? Retraite et Société , 36 (2), 30-49.         [ Links ]

Lorenzi-cioldi, F. (1993). They all look alike and so do we sometimes: Perceptions of ingroup and outgroup homogeneity as a function of sex and context. British Journal of Social Psychology , 32 , 111-124.         [ Links ]

Lorenzi-cioldi, F. (1998). Group status and perceptions of homogeneity. European Review of Social Psychology , 9 (1), 31-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/147927798430000450045         [ Links ]

Mangin, F. (2015). La co-construction des positionnements identitaires des enseignants et des élèves en EPS . These de Doctorat, departement Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives - Laboratoire SPMS. Université de Bourgogne.         [ Links ]

Meier, O. (2008). La gestion des seniors en entreprise. Enjeux et esquisses de solution. Annales Des Mines-Gérer et Comprendre. ESKA , 92 (2), 23-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/geco.092.00230023         [ Links ]

Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In R. Farr & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Social representations . Cambridge/Paris: Cambridge University Press/Maison des Sciences de l'Homme.         [ Links ]

North, M. S., & Fiske, S. T. (2015). Modern attitudes toward older adults in the aging world: A cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin , 141 (5), 993-1021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039469         [ Links ]

Perchot, R. (2013). Racial stacking et stéréotypes raciaux en sport collectif : le cas particulier du basket-ball en contexte français . These de Doctorat, departement Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives - Laboratoire SPMS. Université de Bourgogne.         [ Links ]

Perchot, R., Mangin, F., Lacassagne, M.-F., & Castel, P. (2013). Stacking et discrimination : le cas de deux études en contexte virtuel. Movement & Sport Sciences - Science & Motricité , 79 (1), 75-83. http://doi.org/10.1051/sm/2012033         [ Links ]

Peteuil, A. (2015). Présentation d'une méthode d'étude des relations sociales (RepMut): applications aux rapports entre les malades chroniques et les bien-portants . These de Doctorat, Sciences Humaines et Sociales - Psychologie - Laboratoire SPMS. Université de Bourgogne Franche-Comté         [ Links ].

Sherif, M. (1954). Integrating field work and laboratory in small group research. American Sociological Review , 19 , 759-771. doi:10.2307/2087924         [ Links ]

Simon, B. (1992). The perception of ingroup and outgroup homogeneity: Reintroducing the intergroup context. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (pp. 1-30). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.         [ Links ]

Steinberg, S., & Jégu, M. (2015). Transnationale Instrumente einer generationssensiblen Organisationsentwicklung - Ergebnisse der deutsch-französischen Zusammenarbeit im Projekt DEBBI. En S. Jeschke, A. Richert, F. Hees, & C. Joob (Eds.), Exploring Demographics - Teildisziplinäre Perspektiven zur Innovationsfähigkeit im demografischen Wandel (pp. 517-527). Wiesbaden: Springer.         [ Links ]

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-47). Monterrey, CA: Brooks/Cole.         [ Links ]

Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorisation theory . Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell Pub.         [ Links ]

Van Knippenberg, A. (1989). Strategies of identity management. In P. Van Oudenhoven & T. M. Willemsen (Eds.), Ethnic minorities: Social-psychological perspectives (pp. 59-76). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.         [ Links ]

Velandia-Coustol, C. R., Castel, P., & Lacassagne, M.-F. (2015). Black - White intergroup relations in France. The impact of antiracist norm and categorization process using the RepMut tool. Athens Journal of Social Sciences , 2 (4), 263-274. Recuperado de http://www.athensjournals.gr/social/2015-2-4-2-Coustol.pdf         [ Links ]

Creative Commons License All the contents of this journal, except where otherwise noted, is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License